After providing all the funding for The Brain from Top to Bottom for over 10 years, the CIHR Institute of Neurosciences, Mental Health and Addiction informed us that because of budget cuts, they were going to be forced to stop sponsoring us as of March 31st, 2013.

We have approached a number of organizations, all of which have recognized the value of our work. But we have not managed to find the funding we need. We must therefore ask our readers for donations so that we can continue updating and adding new content to The Brain from Top to Bottom web site and blog.

Please, rest assured that we are doing our utmost to continue our mission of providing the general public with the best possible information about the brain and neuroscience in the original spirit of the Internet: the desire to share information free of charge and with no adverstising.

Whether your support is moral, financial, or both, thank you from the bottom of our hearts!

Bruno Dubuc, Patrick Robert, Denis Paquet, and Al Daigen




Thursday, 23 March 2017
When You Come Into a Room and Forget What You Were Going To Do There

Of all the psychological effects that have been given specific names (the placebo effect, the McGurk effect, the Coolidge effect, etc.), the “doorway effect” is one of the most familiar and yet also one of the most surprising. We have all experienced it: you’re at home, you go from one room into another, and then you forget what you were planning to do there!

As the first article linked to below notes, the French poet Paul Valéry once said that the purpose of psychology is to give us a completely different idea of the things we know best. In this sense, the doorway effect might be considered a perfect example of the kinds of phenomena that psychologists study. And in fact, both this article and the second (from a site that specializes in explaining the psychological mechanisms behind our most familiar behaviours) show how empirical and theoretical data from contemporary psychology shed a different light on this apparent memory failure and instead depict it as a strategy that the human brain uses to adapt to the limited storage capacity of its working memory.

Scientists have known for some time that the memory systems of the human brain are contextual. Thus, it stands to reason that if you decide to perform a task in which you must maintain your intention for a little while, your brain will be subject to the effects of context during that interval. But as Gabriel A. Radvansky and his colleagues showed in their 2011 study, the doorway effect is more than just that. In this study, the subjects played an ingenious video game in which they picked an object up off a table, put the object it in a “virtual backpack” in which they could not see it, then carried it to another table, put the object down, and picked up a different one. In some cases, the subject simply moved from one table to another in the same room, in other cases from a table in one room to a table in another room, and in still others, from a table in one room to a table in another and then back again. In both of the last two cases, the subjects’ answers were slower and less specific when they had gone into another room, even if they had then returned to the original one. These results demonstrated that it was essentially their having gone into the other room (and not whether or not they had returned to the first one) that erased or destabilized their memory.

We can see the potential adaptive value of this phenomenon when we consider the limited capacity of human working memory. When we are taken out of one particular context and immersed in another, we are faced with new challenges and must adapt to them, and one way to do so is to “empty” our working memory so that we can store something new in it. It is of course possible, as well as adaptive, for us to keep our attention on a specific objective despite the change in context, but it demands more “top-down” concentration to offset the “bottom-up” imperatives of the new environment.

* * *

Because my memory, like anyone else’s, is highly associative, writing this post has made me remember an image that I saw recently that illustrated the doorway effect quite specifically. It was one of some 300 images in Here, a sort of graphic novel by Richard McGuire that completely blew me away as soon as I understood what he was doing in it. In Here, everything is viewed from the same point in space (in the present time, a particular corner of the living room of the house in New Jersey where the author grew up in the mid-20th century). But the surprise comes as the ensuing images shift time frames, showing the view from that same point in space at different points in time across many years.

At the start of the book, you understand that you’re in the author’s childhood living room. In some cases, two or three images depict consecutive moments in time. But then the images suddenly leap from the 1950s to the 1980s, then back to the 1960s, then forward to 2005. And then you’re looking at a swamp! Yes, you’re still in the same place, but now, instead of exploring the author’s memories, you’re in the early Neolithic Age (about 10,000 B.C.E.), exploring the memory of our species! Farther along in the book, you go back 50,000 years into the past, and still farther, you are projected into the future: the year 2052. For me, the most striking image for me was the one composed of nothing but greenish clouds, suggesting that I was looking back over 3 billion years, to the origins of life on Earth, but still from the place where the house in which McGuire grew up would be built in 1907.

To get an idea of the variety of the scenes illustrated in this book, enter the string “Here+McGuire” in Google Images or another image search engine. They range from native Americans in a forest glade in the early 1600s to the author’s living room being repainted in the 1990s to the image at the start of this post, where a woman has walked into the living room and is saying to herself, “Hum… Now why did I come in here again?” Many people regard Here as a kind of UFO in the world of graphic novels. I don’t know whether McGuire was familiar with the expression “doorway effect” when he designed it. But if he decided to depict this particular situation, it was because it represents such a common, emblematic phenomenon in human psychology.

i_lien Why Walking through a Doorway Makes You Forget
i_lien Why does walking through doorways make us forget?
a_exp Walking through doorways causes forgetting: Further explorations
i_lien Richard McGuire’s “Here”

Memory and the Brain | No comments


If you have a comment, please e-mail it to me, and I will post it here.